Rehabilitation and Vestibular Compensation

Timothy C. Hain, MDReturn to Index. •Page last modified: August 22, 2020

See also: Anatomy of vestibular compensation

What is vestibular compensation ?

The vestibular system is the inner ear and associated circuits in the nervous system that sense motion of the body (mainly the head), and use this to control movement and posture.

We have two ears, and there are several illnesses where one ear may get shut down, either temporarily or permanently. When this happens, the inner ears become imbalanced, and also do not react fully for a given head movement. There is inappropriate vestibular tone, and incorrect vestibular gain. This causes spontaneous nystagmus and oscillopsia.

Vestibular compensation is the central nervous system process that adapts to these two problems -- getting rid of imbalance in tone, and readjusting gain. These are separate processes that occur with different time courses.

History of vestibular compensation ?

Recovery from vestibular lesions has been observed clinically for over 100 years (Von Bechterew, 1883). Many of the earliest investigations reported on the effects of various lesions on Bechterew (1883) nystagmus, which arises on destruction of the second labyrinth in hemilabyrinthectomized animals. Ewald noted that compensation is upset by blinding (1892). Magnus (1924) demonstrated a transient Bechterew nystagmus in decerebrate animals.  Siegel and Demetriades (1925) reported compensation even after cerebellectomy. Schaefer and Meyer (1974) found that plaster casts applied to the hindlimbs of animals impaired compensation. Similarly compensation is impaired by unilateral cerebral decortication (di Giorgio, 1939), destruction of the inferior olives (Llinas and Walton, 1977), and postbrachial section of the cord (Azzena, 1969; Azzena et al, 1976).    Key work related to adaptive plasticity of the VOR was published in 1976 (Gonshor and Jones, 1976) where it was found that normal animals can even reverse their VOR, given an appropriate demand. 

Experimental models used in humans are generally "experiments of nature", including individuals who have had vestibular nerve section, removal of acoustic neuromas, and canal plugging for treatment of BPPV or Superior Canal Dehiscence (SCD).

Static and dynamic vestibular deficits

After an acute unilateral loss of peripheral vestibular function, there are numerous well known postural and oculomotor deficits that appear. These can be subdivided into static and dynamic deficits -- static signs are present without head movement and dynamic signs are present only during head movement. Static signs include mainly postural asymmetries and spontaneous nystagmus. Dynamic signs include depressed and asymmetrical VOR, and diminished vestibulocular reflexes. Associated with these signs are behavioral changes usually aimed at minimizing the risk of fall by adopting a more cautious stance, and also a reduced propensity to move the head, to avoid dynamic problems.

Four processes involved in vestibular compensation.

Recovery can occur through four processes outlined below

Methods of compensation

Recovery from peripheral vestibular lesions is a complex, multimodal process with the potential for important interactions between components.  These processes are ordinarily all initiated simultaneously, are partially redundant, and with the exception of restoration of peripheral function, they are all competitive.  By this I mean that the success of one process may eliminate the need for another process and thereby reduce its efficacy or development.  This naturally then leads to the conclusion that recovery patterns may be idiosyncratic.  An individual who uses vision and prediction very well might never develop good  brainstem compensation for diminished dynamic vestibular responses.  One then might wonder about optimality : is there one “best” pattern of vestibular compensation that we as clinicians should be striving to foster, or is the body “wise” enough to choose the optimal compensatory strategy in each individual ?   We will next consider each of these processes in more detail.

Process 1: Restoration of peripheral function:

Many  processes that impair vestibular function are time-limited, and recovery occurs because the lesion has disappeared rather than because of any neurological mechanism invoked by the patient. The most common causes of vestibular vertigo are listed below.

Table 1: Proportion of patients that recover spontaneously from vestibular disorders

Condition

Proportion of Otologic Vertigo

Fraction that recover spontaneously

Aggregate proportion that recovers (col 2*col 3)

BPPV (Benign Paroxysmal Positional Vertigo)

50%

80% recover by 6 months

40

Menieres Disease

18%

2 hours-2 days

18

Vestibular neuritis

10%

50% recover by  2 months

5

Bilateral Vestibular Loss

5%

25% recover by 6 months

1

Acoustic Neuroma

1%

Never

0

TOTAL

84

 

49

Note that almost half of patients with vertigo related to vestibular neuritis recover caloric responses spontaneously.  We will develop subsequently the idea that unnecessary vestibular compensation may be harmful. This leads us to our first, counter-intuitive conclusion:

Vestibular compensation is unnecessary (and probably counterproductive) in almost half of all patients with clinical vestibular disorders.

Process 2: Brainstem mechanisms that implement vestibular compensation:

After a unilateral vestibular ablation there are two types of deficits for which a correction is required; a static imbalance related to differences in the levels of tonic discharge within the vestibular nuclei, and a dynamic disturbance, related to the loss of one half of afferent input.

Table 3: Deficits following unilateral vestibular loss

Deficit

Manifestation

CNS Compensation

Static imbalance


Spontaneous nystagmus


Head, body and ocular tilt


Rapid


Complete


Robust


sensory input not needed

Dynamic imbalance  

Movement induced visual disturbance

Ataxia


Slow


Incomplete


Fragile


Needs sensory input

Grossly, one may say that that the static compensation system works very well, and that the dynamic compensation system has significant problems when considering recovery from vestibular lesions. We will now consider these processes individually.

Restoration of static balance:  Immediately after unilateral labyrinthectomy there is complete loss of neural activity on the ipsilateral vestibular nucleus accompanied by increased activity in the contralateral vestibular nucleus (Precht et al, 1966: Mccabe and Ryu, 1969; McCabe et al, 1972). The sudden loss of resting activity relates to loss of peripheral input combined with continued commissural inhibition from the intact side. The increased activity on the contralateral side is presumed due to the removal of commissural inhibition from the lesioned side.  In cat and guinea pig, several weeks after the onset of the injury the mean resting activity of the vestibular nucleus neurons on both sides are almost equal (Markham et al, 1977, 1984; Ris et al, 1995). 

Resolution of spontaneous nystagmus is fast. In rat, guinea pigs and cat, spontaneous nystagmus is gone by 48 hours (Jensen, 1979; Llinas and Walton 1977; Sirkin et al, 1984; Vibert el al, 1993). In guinea pigs, spontaneous nystagmus abates by 50% by 15 hours post lesion even though the deafferented vestibular nucleus neurons have no spontaneous activity (Vidal et al, 1998).  In monkeys the process appears to take a little longer -- Fetter and Zee(1988) found that a 9 deg/sec nystagmus was still present after three months. Furthermore, it was independent of exposure to light. Fisch reported that in humans with vestibular neurectomies, nystagmus was maximal in the first post-operative week but decreased rapidly thereafter, being only 3 deg/sec for the next three years (Fisch, 1973). Thus the compensation process for tone imbalance resulting in nystagmus appears to happen quickly and without the need of sensory input.

A mechanism called the “cerebellar clamp” has been postulated to explain the rapid resolution of spontaneous nystagmus (McCabe et al, 1972). They wrote that “in response to a massive asymmetry of discharges into the cerebellum occasioned by the sudden deafferentation of the vestibular nuclei of one side (by unilateral labyrinthectomy), cerebellar inhibition imposes a shutdown of the intact side, to rebalance the asymmetry at the lowest level”.  Fisch also felt that in patients post-neurectomy, there was strong central inhibition for the first four weeks followed by a more gradual process when central inhibition is gradually reduced. During the period of the cerebellar clamp, the dynamic VOR is necessarily poor.  This may be the reason why Maoli, Precht and Reid found improvements in VOR in lesioned cats occurred much more slowly than adaptive plasticity in normal animals (1983).  Recent research in animals (Balaban) has shown that there is increased metabolic activity in the inferior olive and vestibulocerebellum (posterior vermis) 6 hours after unilateral nerve section. This may be the equivalent of the “cerebellar clamp”. This increase in metabolic activity persists chronically and presumably reflects an internal re-balancing of the vestibular brainstem related to internal feedback loops. The vestibular commissures do not appear to be necessary for static compensation to occur or its maintenance.

Static compensation is robust.  Resolution of spontaneous nystagmus occurs independently of visual input in monkeys (Fetter and Zee, 1988) and is also little affected by somatosensory input in baboons and guinea pigs (Lacour et al, 1976;Schaefer and Meyer, 1975). Potential substrates for static adaptation include the denervation hypersensitivity to vestibular input in the vestibular nucleus (Curthoys and Halmagyi, 1996), greater reliance on commissures (Dieringer and Straka, 1998), and the activity deriving from the deep cerebellar nuclei and the inferior olive (Zee, 1994). Because cats with labyrinthectomy recover more quickly than those with unilateral loss induced by  neurectomy,  restoration of resting activity may also derive from surviving primary vestibular neurons (Cass and Goshgarian, 1991).  However, human studies have not supported the idea that labyrinthectomy patients do any better than nerve sections (Takemori et al, 1984).

Postural deviation appears to be mainly derived from asymmetrical otolith activity. In frogs, postural symptoms are attributed primarily to the asymmetry in utricular input (Dieringer and Strata, 1998). Head posture normalizes with a time constant of about 21 days, unrelated to whether the lesion is pre or post-ganglionic.  There are substantial differences among species. Rabbits do not recover a normal head posture after hemilabyrinthectomy, while rodents regain a normal head posture within 3 days and humans barely exhibit a postural deficit of the head at all (Dieringer N, 1995).  In the guinea pig head deviation is also related to asymmetries in otolith input.

Section of the vestibular commissures does not impair postural vestibular compensation in guinea pig (Smith et al, 1986) suggesting that commissural activity does not play a strong role in static compensation in this animal. Postural normalization is closely correlated with recovery of resting discharge in 2nd order vestibular neurons, which occurs in roughly 2 days (Ris et al, 1995). However in frogs, postural vestibular compensation (see following paragraph) does appear to be related to increased synaptic efficacy of commissural fibers to the sectioned side (Dieringer and Straka, 1998).The compensation process for tone imbalance resulting in postural deviation may also depend on contributions from somatosensory inputs in animals (Jensen, 1979; Schaefer and Meyer, 1975).


Table 4:


Recovery of Dynamic VOR to hemi-labyrinthine lesion


Animal Species


Findings


Reference

Rabbit

  Barsma and Collewign, 1974

Cat

  Maoli et al, 1983


Maoli and Precht, 1985

Rat

Deficient maculo and canal-ocular reflexes at 1 year

Hamann et al, 1998

Guinea pig

2/3 normal by 1 month

Vibert et al, 1993


Monkey

Symmetric at 3 mo for 30 deg/sec, asym. at 240


Symmetric at 1 mo. for 120 deg/sec, asymmetric at 240

Fetter and Zee, 1988


Takahashi et al, 1977


Wolfe and Kos, 1977


Paige, 1983


Human

  Baloh et al, 1984


Takahashi et al, 1984


Allum et al, 1988


Maas et al, 1989


Paige 1989


Fetter and Dichgans, 1990

Restoration of dynamic VOR.  Recovery has been studied in the rabbit, cat, guinea pig, monkey, and human. Fetter and Zee studied restoration of VOR gain after unilateral labyrinthectomy in monkeys (1988). There was no increase in gain until exposure to light. Furthermore, improvements are lost after bilateral occipital lobectomy. It has previously been shown that dynamic adaptive changes are also lost after removal of the vestibulocerebellum (Robinson, 1976; Haddad et al, 1977).  These results suggest that adaptation of VOR gain is a dynamic process that requires visual experience for its acquisition and is dependent on CNS structures for  maintenance (Zee, 1994).   Potential substrates for mediation of dynamic adaptation include the flocculus, inferior olive, and vestibular commissures (Galiana et al, 1984).  Precht showed that cutting of the vestibular commissure  in cat eliminates rotational sensitivity of ipsilateral type-I cells showing that recovery of in cat dynamic VOR probably requires commissar participation.

In humans, dynamic compensation appears to take on the order of months, as opposed to the quicker process of static compensation.

On a neural basis, it is clear that recovery of the dynamic VOR requires recovery of vestibular neuron sensitivity to velocity and acceleration. There are several mechanisms. A component of recovery clearly relates to restoration of tonic firing on the ipsilateral side (for labyrinthectomy and nerve section), or in other words, should be correlated with loss of the tonic offset.  This mechanism works in essence because more neurons are active, being out of inhibitory cutoff, and also implies a direction dependent gain nonlinearity, as is observed in behavioral data. This could, in theory, compensate only up to a 50% VOR level, as after labyrinthectomy, half of the input is gone. This mechanism is supported experimentally by many studies (e.g. Smith and Curthoys, 1988).

A second component relates to increased sensitivity of central neurons, as clearly with only half of the input remaining, for recovery to occur, central sensitivity must double. For sensitivity to increase on the side ipsilateral to the lesion, information must necessarily be transferred from the intact side to the lesioned side, presumably via the vestibular commissures. Such increases in sensitivity have been observed (Hamann and Lannou, 1988; Newlands and Perachio, 1989)

Table 5: Cell types in the vestibular nucleus, according to firing characteristics

Cell type

Characteristics

Reference

Vestibular only (VO)

Vestibular input

Fuchs and Kimm, 1975

Vestibular-pause (VP)

Vestibular and pause during saccades

Fuchs and Kimm, 1975

Keller and Kamath, 1975

Tomlinson and Robinson, 1984

Position-vestibular-pause (PVP)

Eye position, vestibular input, pause

Tomlinson and Robinson, 1984

Scudder and Fuchs, 1992

Eye-head velocity cells

eye velocity minus head velocity (gaze velocity)

Scudder and Fuchs, 1992

Floccular target neurons (FTN)

PVP like cells that diminish responsiveness during vestibular suppression

Lisberger et al, 1994

There are many subpopulations of neurons in the vestibular nucleus (see table above). It is presently unclear the degree to which each population is altered during the process of vestibular compensation, but Lisberger and colleagues (1984) have argued that the FTN are the critical modifiable element in VOR plasticity. If true, the FTN may also be the modifiable element in vestibular dynamic compensation.  If the plasticity pathways are also used for compensation (which would be reasonable), one would expect that after vestibular compensation, plasticity may be “used up”. This concept was tested by Maoli and Precht (1985), who reported that additional plastic change induced by visual stimuli was possible even after labyrinthectomy, although cats with labyrinthectomy compensated much more slowly than normal animals (Maoli, Precht and Reid, 1983).

Dynamic compensation, at least driven by the labyrinth, may never be perfect following hemilabyrinthectomy. Typically recovery is best for low velocities or accelerations, but fails for higher velocities/accelerations, suggesting saturation phenomena (Halmagyi et al, 1990; Paige, 1983).

Table 6: What Amount of Compensation do we need ?

Condition

Lesion

Need from CNS

BPPV (Benign Paroxysmal Positional Vertigo)

1 canal hyperactive

Suppress 1 canal

Menieres Disease

Episodic imbalance

Transient suppression

of one inner ear

Vestibular neuritis

0-50% loss

1-2x Gain

Bilateral Vestibular Loss

50-100% loss

2x-infinite gain

Acoustic Neuroma

50% loss

2x gain

 

Table 7: How much plasticity do we have available ?

Need

Lesion

Response

Species

Authors

2x

Spectacle         

1.7-1.9x           

Monkey           

Miles and Eighmy

2x

lat. Canal plug

1.6x

Monkey

Paige, 1983

2x

lat. Canal plug

0

Cat      

Baker et al, 1982

2x

labyrinthectomy

1.5x (estimate) 

Cat

Cass and Goshgarian, 1991

Adaptive Plasticity in the VOR was illustrated in a dramatic fashion in an experiment of Gonshor and Melvill Jones (1976). They forced the VOR to reverse itself by rotating human subjects with mirror-reversed vision ("Dove" prisms). Thus when a subject was experiencing right-going vestibular stimulation, instead of the usual left-going eye movements of the normal VOR, the eyes would have to move to the right in the head for image stabilization. For this paradigm there was a progressive maintained attenuation of VOR gain which was carried over to the next day. After several weeks using reversing dove prisms, it was shown that a VOR reappeared in approximately the reverse direction (Gonshor and Melvill Jones, 1976)

Miles and colleagues, using monkeys, continued to study this issue. Using magnifying or minifying spectacles, aimed at asking for augmentation or attenuation, they were able to document adaptive changes of the VOR gain up to 1.8X, for 2.0 glasses.  The relevance to studies in normal animals to animals with lesions was questioned by Maoli, Precht and Reid (1983) who reported that cats with unilateral lesions compensated much more slowly than normal animals.

Central mechanisms: It is clear that adaptive plasticity is obtained through a combination of the action of the cerebellum and the brainstem. A direct brainstem pathway exists between the vestibular nuclei and the oculomotor nuclei.  Adaptive VOR changes could be mediated via either changes in the properties of this direct pathway, or through indirect side-pathways that augment or overwhelm the direct pathway.

Table 8: Evidence for critical period

Critical Period

Present ?

Reference

Postural Control

Yes (Baboon)

Lacour et al, 1989

Postural control

Yes (Baboon)

Lacour et al, 1976

Postural Control

Yes (Guinea pigs)

Jensen, 1979

     

VOR – low velocity

No (Monkey)

Fetter and Zee, 1988

VOR – high velocity

Yes

Fetter and Zee, 1988

     

Spontaneous nystagmus

No (Baboon)

Lacour et al, 1976

Spontaneous nystagmus

No (Guinea pigs)

Jensen, 1979

Critical period ? Some investigators have reported that unless visual experience is allowed early after a labyrinthine lesion, recovery is suboptimal (Lacour et al,  1989). Fetter and Zee (1988) did not generally find this as monkeys kept in the dark for 4 days after lesion and then allowed normal visual experience, generally showed recovery of dynamic VOR at about the same rate as monkeys who were kept in the dark for the first days. There was one important exception – for high-velocity rotations toward the lesioned side, recovery was delayed in monkeys initially deprived of vision. There appears to be more evidence for a critical period in postural control than in the VOR (Zee, 1994).  According to Herdman and associates (1995), it is still unclear whether there is a critical period in humans for recovery from acute vestibular imbalance, but exercises early after acoustic neuroma resection facilitate the rate of recovery of postural stability.

Context Specificity: VOR gain adaptation is context specific. Baker and associates have showed that cats can be trained to change the gain of their VOR differently with one ear down than when the other ear is down. This demonstrates that VOR adaptation can be changed by static otolithic signals.

Process 3: Substitution of responses based on alternative inputs

Potential alternative sources of input to stabilize vision or posture AFTER A VESTIBULAR LESION

Vision

Other vestibular organs (i.e. otoliths or vertical canals for lateral canals).

Somatosensory – COR (neck-eye loop)

Prediction

Changes in cognitive processing – i.e. suppression of oscillopsia

Saccadic substitution (Vestibular catch-up saccades)

Voluntary modulation of the VOR and COR

There are a variety of possible sensory inputs that can be used to substitute for lost vestibular input (Courjon et al, 1982; Putkonen et al, 1977). Visual tracking is well known to be used as a substitute for vestibular input, although smooth pursuit fails at frequencies above 0.5 Hz.

There is considerable evidence that the brain can use other canals to substitute for unilateral canal plugs (Bohmer et al, 1982), such as are used to treat SCD and occasional refractory BPPV.  The brain can also apparently use otolith inputs. Bohmer et al (1982, 1985), studied canal-plugged monkeys and found recovery of VOR at high frequency sinusoids, with a gain near normal at 4 hz. They attributed this recovery to preserved otolithic information. 

Cervical inputs are also a well known source for an incomplete compensatory response resembling the VOR. Baker et al (1982) studied canal plugged cats and reported increase of the COR gain to about 0.1, two weeks post-lesion.   Dichgans and associates showed that in monkeys with bilateral labyrinthectomy, the COR grows to a gain of about 0.3 at 7 weeks post-lesion (1973).  The same authors also concluded that these animals could modulate the gain of their COR during active head movements.  The situation appears to be more complicated in humans although it has been studied occasionally (Bronstein and Hood, 1986; Jurgens and Mergner, 1989; Kasai and Zee, 1978), and COR measurements appear very dependent on instruction suggesting either voluntary modulation or prediction.  Recently clinicians have noticed that  vibration of neck muscles reliably creates a nystagmus in persons with unilateral loss. This likely represents another compensatory pathway, which has so far been little studied.

Prediction or anticipation seems particularly likely to be a mechanism of recovery for individuals who are vestibularly impaired.  It is well known that the VOR gain can be modulated, or in other words,  enhanced or suppressed, by voluntary effort (Barr et al, 1976). There are also suggestions that the COR gain can be modulated (Kasai and Zee, 1978). In animals, use of prediction has been postulated in bilateral canal plugged cats  (Baker et al, 1982) and monkeys with bilateral labyrinthectomy (Dichgans et al, 1973). In the latter study, the combination of the COR with prediction enabled the animals to obtain a total gain of 90% at seven weeks, but only with active head movements. 

Saccadic substitution or supplementation involves production of saccades in the direction of the VOR (or in other words, oppositely directed to fast-phases which oppose the VOR), in an attempt to maintain fixation on a visual target (Segal and Katsarkas, 1988). As vision is suppressed during saccades, the point of this putative mechanism must be to put the eye on the object of regard after the saccade is over. It has been well documented in subsequent studies where it was termed "VCUS" (Tian et al, 2000). Recent investigators have renamed this phenomenon "covert" vs. "overt" saccades.

Physical therapy, which frequently involves use of voluntary head or body movement, presumably promotes use of substitution, modulation and prediction. One would expect that this recovery mechanism would be best facilitated by natural activities, such as occur while walking or climbing, and might be less well promoted by special sensory arrangements.

Process 4: Behavioral changes which minimize the impact of vestibular disturbance

Like substitution, little attention has also been paid to adaptive strategies which decrease the need for vestibular input. If head movement can be limited in frequency, visual tracking mechanisms may be adequate to substitute for an absent or reduced VOR. If challenges to balance can be minimized, risk of falling or at least mis-stepping can be reduced. Berthoz (1985) observed reduction of rapid head movements to the side of vestibular injury.  These mechanisms  seem likely to be those most facilitated by physical therapy.

References

  • Alice C, Paul AE, Sansom AJ, Maclennan K, Darlington C, Smith P. The effects of steroids on vestibular compensation and vestibular nucleus neuronal activity in the guinea pig. J. Vest Res, 8,3,201-207, 1998
  • Allum JHJ, Yamane M, Pfaltz CR. Long-term modifications of vertical and horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex dynamics in man. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 105:328-337, 1988
  • Azzena GB, 1969. Role fo the spinal cord in compensating the effects of hemilabyrinthectomy. Arch Ital Biol 107, 43
  • Azzena GB, Manoli O, Tolu E. 1976. Vestibular nuclei of hemilabyrinthectomized guinea pigs during decompensation. Arch Ital Biol 114: 389
  • Baker J, Goldberg J, Peterson B, Schor R. Oculomotor reflexes after semicircular canal plugging in cats. Brain Res 252, 151-155, 1982
  • Baker J, Wickland C, Peterson B. Dependence of cat vestibulo-ocular reflex direction adaptation on animal orientation during adaptation and rotation in darkness. Brain Res, 408 (1987), 339-343
  • Baarsma EZ, Collewign H. Changes in compensatory eye movements after unilateral
  • labyrinthectomy in the rabbit. Arch Otorhinolaryngol 211:219-230, 1974
  • Baloh RW, Honrubia V, Yee RD, Hess K. Changes in human vestibulo-ocular reflex after loss of peripheral sensitivity. Ann Neurol 16:222-228, 1984
  • Barr CC, Schultheis LW, Robinson DA. Voluntary, non-visual control of the human vestibulo-ocular reflex. Acta Otolaryngol 81:365-375, 1976
  • Bechterew W. 1883. Ergebnisse der Durchschneidung des N. Acusticus, nebts Eroterung der Bedeutung der semicircularen Canale fur das Korpergleichwicht. Pflugers Arch Ges Physiol 30, 312
  • Berthoz A. Adaptive mechanisms in eye-head coordination. In A. Berthoz and G. melville Jones (ed). Adaptive mechanisms in gaze control. Elsevier, Amsterdam, 177-203, 1985
  • Bohmer A, Henn V, Suzuki J. Compensatory eye movement in the monkey during high-frequency sinusoids. In: Physiological and Pathological Aspects of Eye Movements (Roucoux A and Crommelinck N Eds, 1982, Junk, The Hague.
  • Bohmer A, Henn V, Suzuki J. Vestibulo-ocular reflexes after elective plugging of the semicirular canals in the monkeys ? response plane determinations. Brain Res 326:1985,291-298
  • Bronstein Am, Hood JD. The cervico-ocular reflex in normal subjects and patients with absent vestibular function. Brain Res. 373. 399-408, 1986.
  • Cass SP, Goshgarian HG. Vestibular compensation after labyrinthectomy and vestibular neurectomy in cats. Otol HNS 1991:104:14
  • Courjon JH, Flandrin JM. Jeannerod M, Schmid R. The role of the flocculus in vestibular compensation after hemilabyrinthectomy. Brain Res, 239:257-257, 1982
  • Demetriades TD, Speigel EA. 1927. Zur Frage des Bedeutung des Kleinhirns fur die Entwicklung von Spontannystagmus Z. Hals-, Nas-Ohrenheilkd 19, 250.
  • Dichgans J, Bizzi E, Morasso P, Tagliasco . Mechanisms underlying recovery of eye-head coordination following bilateral labyrinthectomy in monkeys. Exp Br. Res, 18, 548-562, 1973
  • Dieringer N. Vestibular compensation: neural plasticity and its relations to functional recovery after labyrinthine lesions in frogs and other vertebrates. Prog Neurobiol 1995:46:97-129.
  • Dieringer N, Straka H. Steps toward recovery of function after hemilabyrinthectomy in frogs. Otolaryngol HNS 1998:119:27-33
  • Di Giorgio AM. 1939. Effecti di lesioni unilaterali della corteccia cerebral sui fenomeni di compenso da emislabirintazione. Atti Fisiol Fac Med Siena Ser XI:2, 382
  • Ewald JR. 1982. Physiologische Untersuchungen uber das Endorgan des Nervus Octavus. Bergman, Wiesbaden.
  • Fetter M, Zee DS, Proctor LR. Effect of lack of vision and occipital lobectomy upon recovery from unilateral labyrinthectomy in rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 59:394, 1988
  • Fisch U. The vestibular response following unilateral vestibular neurectomy. Acta Otol 76:229-238, 1973
  • Fuchs and Kimm. Unit activity in vestibular nucleus of the alert monkey during horizontal angular acceleration and eye movement. J. Neurophysiol 72:909-927, 1975
  • Galiana HL, Flohr H, Melvill Jones G. A re-evaluation of intervestibular nuclear coupling: its role in vestibular compensation. J Neurophysiol 51(1984), 242-259
  • Gonshor A, Jones MG. Extreme vestibulo-ocular adaptation induced by prolonged optical reversal of vision. J. Physiol 256: (1976) 381-414. Classic article on potential for plasticity.
  • Haddad GM, Friendlich AR, Robinson DA. Compensation of nystagmus after VIIIth lesions in vestibulo-cerebellectomized cats. Brain Res 135, 192-196, 1977
  • Halmagyi GM, Curthoys IS and others. The human horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to high-acceleration stimulation before and after unilateral vestibular neurectomy. EBR 81:479-490, 1990
  • Halmagyi M and Curthoys I. How does the brain compensate for vestibular lesions ? In: Baloh and Halmagyi (ed), Disorders of the Vestibular System. Oxford, 1996
  • Hamman KF, Lannou J. Dynamic characteristics of vestibular nuclear neurons responses to vestibular and optokinetic stimulation during vestibular compensation in the rat. Acta Otol Suppl 455:1-19.
  • Hamann KF, Reber A, Hes BJM et al. Long-term deficits in otolith, canal and optokinetic ocular reflexes of pigmented rats after unilateral vestibular nerve section. Exp Br. Res 1998:118:331-40
  • Herdman SJ, Clendianiel RA, Mattox DE, Holliday MJ, Niparko JK. Vestibular adaptation exercises and recovery: acute stage after acoustic neuroma resection. Otol HNS 1995:113:77-87
  • Jensen DW. Reflex control of acute postural asymmetry and compensatory symmetry after a unilateral vestibular lesion. Neuroscience 4:1059, 1979
  • Jurgens R, Mergner T. Interaction between cervico-ocular and vestibulo-ocular reflexes in normal adults. EBR. 77:381-390, 1989
  • KasaiT, Zee DS. eye-head coordination in labyrinthine defective human beings. Brain Res. 144: 123-141, 1978
  • Lacour M, Roll JP, Appaix M. Modifications and development of spinal reflexes in the alert baboon (Papio papio) following an unilateral vestibular neurotomy. Brain Res 113,255-269, 1976
  • Lacour M et al. Vestibular Compensation: Facts, Theories, and Clinical Perspectives. Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1989
  • Llinas R, Walton K. Significance of the olivo-cerebellar system in compensation of ocular position following unilateral labyrinthectomy. In: Baker R, Berthoz A (Eds). Control of gaze by brainstem neurons, pp 399-408, 1977
  • Maas EF, Huebner Wp, Seidman SH, Leigh RJ. Behavior of human horizonta vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to high-acceleration stimuli. Brain Res 499:153-156, 1989
  • Magnus R. 1924. Korperstellung. Springer, Berlin
  • Maoli C, Precht W, Reid S. Short and long-term modifications of vestibulo-ocular respond dynamics following unilateral vestibular nerve lesions in the cat. Exp Br. Res, 1983, 50:259-74
  • Maioli C, Precht W. On the role of vestibulo-ocular reflex plasticity in recovery after unilateral peripheral vestibular lesions. Exp Brain Res 59:267-272, 1983
  • Markham CH, yagi T, Curthoys IS. the contribution of the contralateral labyrinth to second order vestibular neural activity in the cat.Brain Res, 138:99-109, 1977
  • Markham CH, Yagi T. Brainstem changes in vestibular compensation. Acta Otol (Stock) (Suppl) 1984 (Suppl 406:83-6).
  • Martin, J., et al. (1996). "Early diazepam treatment following unilateral labyrinthectomy does not impair vestibular compensation of spontaneous nystagmus in guinea pig." J Vestib Res 6(2): 135-139.
  • McCabe BF, Ryu JH. Experiments on vestibular compensation. Laryngoscope 1969:79:1728-36
  • McCabe BF, Ryu JH, Sekitani T. Further experiments on vestibular compensation. Laryngoscope 1972;82:381-96
  • Melvill Jones G. How and why does the vestibulo-ocular reflex adapt ? In: Baloh and Halmagyi (ed), Disorders of the Vestibular System. Oxford, 1996
  • Newlands SD, Perachio AA. Compensation of horizontal canal related activity in the medial vestibular nucleus following unilateral labyrinth ablation in the decerebrate gerbil. I. Type I neurons (EBR 82:359-372).
  • Newlands SD, Perachio AA. Compensation of horizontal canal related activity in the medial vestibular nucleus following unilateral labyrinth ablation in the decerebrate gerbil. II. Type II neurons (EBR 82: 373-383).
  • Paige GD. Nonlinearity and asymmetry in the human vestibulo-ocular reflex. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 108:1-8, 1989
  • Paige GD. Vestibulo-ocular reflex and its interactions with visual following mechanisms in the squirrel monkey. II. Response characteristics and plasticity following unilateral inactivation of the horizontal canal. J. Neurophysiol. 49:152-168, 1983
  • Precht W, Shimazu H, Markham C. A mechanism of central compensation of vestibular function following hemilabyrinthectomy. J. Neurophysiol 1966:29:996-1010[TH4]
  • Putkonen PT, COurjon JH, Jeannerod M. compensation of postural effects of hemilabyrinthectomy in the cat. A sensory substitution process ? Exp Br Res 28:249-257, 1977
  • Ris L, deWaele C, Serafin M, Vidal PP, Godaux E. Neuronal activity in the ipsilateral vestibular nucleus following unilateral labyrinthectomy in the alert guinea pig. J. Neurophysiol. 74:2087-2099, 1995
  • Robinson DA. Adaptive gain control of vestibuloocular reflex by the cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol 1976, 954-
  • Schaeffer KP and Meyer DL. Compensation of vestibular lesions. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol VI, Part 2 (ed. Kornhuber HH), 463-490, Springer NY.
  • Schaefer KP and Meyer DL. Compensatory mechanisms following labyrinthine lesions in the guinea pig. A simple model of learning. In Memory and Transfer of Information (ed. Zippel HP), p. 203-232, Plenum Press, NY.
  • Scudder CA, Fuchs AF. Physiology and behavioral identification of vestibuar nucleus neurons mediating the horizontal vestibuloocular reflex in trained rhesus monekys. J. Neurophysiol 68:244-264, 1992
  • Segal BN, Katsarkas A. Long-term deficits of goal-directed vestibulo-ocular function following total unilateral loss of peripheral vestibular function. Acta Otolaryngol 1988, 106:102-10
  • Sirkin DW, Precht W, Courjon JH. Initial, rapid phase of recovery from unilateral vestibular lesion in rat not dependent on survival of central portion of vestibular nerve. Brain Res., 302:245-256, 1984
  • Smith PF, Darlington CL, Curthoys IS. Vestibular compensation without brainstem commissures in the guinea pig. Neurosci Letters (1986), 209-213
  • Smith PF, darlington CL, Curthoys IS. The effect of visual deprivation in vestibular compensation in the guinea pig. Brain Res 364:195-198, 1986
  • Smith PF, Curthoys IS. Neuronal activity in the ipsilateral medial vestibular nucleus of the guinea pig following unilateral labyrinthectomy. Brain Res 444:308-319, 1988
  • Smith PF, Curthoys IS. Neuronal activity in the contralateral medial vestibular nucleus of the guinea pig following unilateral labyrinthectomy. Brain Res 444:295-297, 1988
  • Takahashi M, Igarachi M, Wright W. damped pendular rotatation nystagmus after unilateral labyrinthectomy or unilateral lateral semicircular canal block in squirrel monkeys. J. Otol 6:157-165, 1977
  • Takahashi M, Uemura T, Fujishiro T. recovery of vestibulo-ocular reflex and gaze disturbances in patients ith unialteral loss of labyrinthine function. Ann ORL 93:170-175, 1984
  • Takemori S, Maeda T, Seki Y, Aiba T. Vestibular compensation after sudden loss of inner ear or vestibular nerve functions. Acta Otol (Stockh) 1984, Suppl 406, 91-94
  • Tian JR, Crane BT, Demer JL. Vestibular catch-up saccades in labyrinthine deficiency. Exp Br. Res 131:448-457, 2000
  • Tomlinson RD, Robinson DA. Signals in the vestibular nuclei mediating vertical eye movements in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol 51:1121-1136, 1984
  • Vibert N, deWaele C, Escudero M, Vidal PP. The horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex in the hemilabyrinthectomized guinea pig. Exp Br Res 97:263-273, 1993
  • Vidal PP, De Waele C, Vibert N, Muhlethaler M. Vestibular compensation revisited. Otolaryngol HNS 1998:119:34-42
  • Von Bechterew W. Ergenbnisse det durchschneidung des n acusticus, benst eroterung der bedeutung der semicircularen kanale fur das korpergleichgewicht. Pflugers Arch 1983:30:312-47
  • Zee DS. Vestibular Adaptation in Vestibular Rehabilitation (Herdman S, Ed). F.A. Davis, 1994